Color Constancy and Contextual Effects on Color Appearance

Color Constancy

how constant descriptors of surface
color are extracted from variable sensory signals

Color Induction

how changes in background properties affect the color appearance of a target stimulus

inverse problem

visual system faces the challenge of parsing the retinal light signal into object and illumination components

computational models

needs prior assumptions

relied on the fact that natural surface reflectances and daylight illuminants can be represented with a limited number of basis functions

assuming certain regularities (e.g. mean chromaticity is neutral) in the visual scene

could estimate the illuminant in a simple scene but might not apply to human color constancy

hypothesis of color constancy

adaptation to mean luminance

adaptation to local contrast

adaptation to the brightest surface in the scene

results showed a combination of several mechanisms

observers used information from several cues, such as highlights, cast shadows, and depth cues when estimating surface color

research paradigm

measuring changes in color appearance caused by a change in viewing context similar to simultaneous contrast illusion

quantify the “compensation” for the difference in surrounds in terms of color constancy, or color constancy index

hypothesis

Helmholtz

simultaneous contrast effect is produced by an error of judgement, where the target is mistakenly assumed to be illuminated with a light having the color of the surround

Due to the discounting of the illumination, the perceived color of the target shifts in a direction opposite in color space to the color of the surround

Hering

both color constancy and simultaneous contrast result from lateral inhibition between neighboring receptors at the retina

lead to assumption in later works that simultaneous contrast is due to mechanisms subserving color constancy that is broadly adopted in the field

HOWEVER, any given observable induction effect may be due to a host of different mechanisms, subserving different functional goals

Type I and Type II Constancy

challenge

we hardly experience the strong changes in perceived when an object moves across a multicolored background

Against the idea that simultaneous contrast and color constancy are due to mechanisms encoding contrast information at the borders between surfaces

Type I constancy

the mechanisms that provide color constancy across illumination changes

Type II constancy

mechanisms that provide color constancy across background changes (background independence)

computational model

Initially, color difference is coded at edges (differentiation)

The concatenation of these two operations yields the original image up to an unknown additive constant (determined by some kind of anchoring rule)

The second stage integrates color difference across space (integration)

simultaneous contrast effects (a failure of background independence) are to be understood as failures of integration

raise emphasis on

how to classify reflectance and illuminant edges

how the postulated integration works

quantify contextual effects

Asymmetric Color Matching

Two target patches embedded in different surrounds

The observer adjusts the color coordinates of one of the targets to match the appearance of the other

offer a direct link to computational models of constancy that estimate the illuminant

Cannot decompose the combined effect of two surrounds into single surround-specific effects without relying on theoretical assumptions (such as neutral surround)

Asymmetric Matching with HSD

Haploscopically Superimposed Displays (HSD)
one target-surround stimulus is presented separately to each eye such that the observer experiences the two targets to be matched as embedded in the same surround

Subjective matching problem seems to be absent compared to conventional paradigm

Achromatic Settings

the observer adjusts the chromaticity of a target embedded in a surround such that it appears achromatic

Obviate the need for presenting a second comparison target

measurements can only be made for a small subset of all possible colors

Unique Hue Settings

based on a notion of opponent color theory that the four unique hues have special properties that make them as landmarks in color space

measurements can be made not only for targets that appear achromatic in a given surround, but also for targets that appear unique red, green, blue, or yellow

interpretation and modeling requires more assumptions than achromatic settings because the set of colors that appear in a given unique hue is a two-dimensional manifold

Threshold and Scaling Measurements

measure color discrimination by threshold measurements or supra-threshold technique

threshold or scaling measurements can be used to estimate the derivative of the color code f(t,s) with respect to t, which cannot be determined based on asymmetric matching

Assumptions in Measuring and Modeling Color Appearance

The Continuity Assumption
perceived color is a continuous function of the color coordinates of the target and the surround

Functionally Neutral Surrounds
Some surrounds (e.g. complete dark) are considered “neutral” to serve as the matching target

The Compensation Assumption
the observer could compensate the net effect of the surrounds on the perceived colors of the targets by adjusting the tristimulus values of one of the targets

Assume the perceived color space is 3-D

The theoretical color space could be up to 6-D
(3 values for target and surround each)

there is often a discontinuity in color appearance when the target color coordinates reach the coordinates of the surround

crispening
color discrimination is best for target colors close to the surround color