SUSS PSY 207 STUDY UNIT 6

CHAPTER 12.1 AND 12.2 Gender Roles
and Sexuality

CHAPTER 14.1 and 14.2 Emotions, Attachment,
and Social Relationships

Sex and Gender

The Infant

Gender Roles and
Stereotypes

Gender Differences or Similarities?

  • Men hold a masculine gender identity, whereas most women have a feminine gender identity.
  • Boys and girls engage in different play activities.
  • Females sometimes display greater verbal abilities than males, but on most verbal tasks the difference is small.
  • Males outperform females on many tests of spatial ability.
  • Females obtain slightly higher math grades in the classroom than males.
  • Girls display greater memory ability than boys. Males engage in more physical and verbal aggression than females

Differential Treatment

  • Gender categorisation occurs upon birth through agents of socilisation which issue differential treatment based on gender.
  • i.e. Baby boys are given a blue baby blanket and baby girls are given a pink baby blanket.
  • Influences the development of masculinity and femininity temperament.

CHAPTER 14.3 to14.5 Emotions, Attachment,
and Social Relationships

The Child

Emotional
Development

First Emotions and
Emotion Regulation

Adolescents’ Emotional
Lives

  • Adolescents have been viewed as moody and emotionally volatile

Emotions and Aging

  • Older adults lead rich and rewarding emotional lives.
  • They are able to experience and express emotions fully but also regulate them effectively to optimize positive feelings and minimize negative ones.

Perspectives on
Relationships

Attachment Theory

Peers: The Second World
of Childhood

The Adolescent

The Infant

Quality of Attachment

Implications of Early
Attachment

Attachment Forms

Play

  • Play is generally defined as activities that do not have an obvious or direct purpose or use

Peer Acceptance

  • Researchers study peer-group acceptance through sociometric techniques—methods for determining who is liked and who is disliked in a group

Peer Networks

  • Children become increasingly dependent on peers for social and emotional support
  • 10% of social interactions as toddlers and 30% of social interactions in middle childhood are with peers

Development in Parent–Child Attachments

  • Parent-child attachment becomes a goal-corrected partnership
    • Parent and child accommodate each other’s needs
    • The child becomes a more sensitive partner and grows more independent of the parent
  • Young children want separations to be predictable and controllable (Controlled proximity maintanence)
  • Continue to seek attention from parents and turn to parents when frightened or hurt (Safe haven)

Friendships

  • adolescents increasingly choose friends with matching psychological qualities—interests, attitudes, values, and personalities

Changing Social Networks

  • Dunphy listed five steps to illustrate the progression from peer relations to romantic attachments

Attachments to Parents

  • Adolescents who have secure attachment relationships with their parents generally display:

Dating

  • Adolescent romantic relationships evolve through the following four phases
  • Primary Emotions (0-6months) very young infants express distinct emotions in response to different experiences (i.e. Distress or Joy)
  • Secondary or Self-conscious emotions (18 months) on Upon developing self-awareness, the infant is able to conduct social comparisons between their behaviour and social standards (i.e. family/ preschool environment). They may display the self-conscious emotions that involve evaluating the self: pride, shame, and guilt
  • Emotion regulation—the processes involved in initiating, maintaining, and altering emotional responses.
  • i.e. 18–24 months, toddlers will try to control whatever is upsetting them by, say, pushing a noisy mechanical toy away

Emotional Learning in
Childhood

  • While temperaments may be genetically rooted, caregivers may influence infants’ predominant patterns of emotional expression.
  • i.e. Mothers respond selectively to their babies’ expressions; over the early months, they become increasingly responsive to their babies’ expressions of happiness, and less responsive to their negative emotions
  • Social referencing. At around 9 months, infants also begin to monitor their social circle for emotional reactions to stimuli and use this information to decide how they should feel and behave.
  • Emotional competence; the display of refined, characteristic patterns of emotional expression, greater understanding of emotion, and better emotion regulation skills.
  • Early emotional competence is a good predictor of social competence

Emotional display rules

  • As children get older, they also learn about emotional display rules—Sociocultural rules specifying what emotions should and should not be expressed under what circumstances
  • (i.e. crying at a funeral is socially acceptable but laughing at a funeral isn’t)

Difficulty in regulating emotions

  • One reason is that adolescents experience more negative life events than children. Because they are experiencing more emotional arousal at a time when the executive control centres of their brains have not fully developed, they can also have more difficulty than adults regulating their emotions.

Contra-hedonic

  • Adolescence displayed contra-hedonic- motives as thet, consciously or unconsciously appear to lean towards maintaining or enhancing their bad moods or dampen their good ones.
  • Adults were the most “prohedonic” of all age groups, often reporting that they were trying to maintain positive affect and dampen negative affect

Amalgamation of emotions

  • Why do adolesence lean to wards bad mood?
  • Adolescence often reported positive and negative emotions simultaneously and seemed to crave being able to combine the two, as in being afraid at a horror movie while enjoying the scary experience with friends,

Socioemotional selectivity theory

  • According to this view, the perception that one has little time left to live prompts aging adults to put less emphasis on the goal of acquiring information for future use and more emphasis on the goal of fulfilling current emotional needs. As a result, older adults narrow their range of social partners to those who bring them emotional pleasure.

Positivity effect

  • Older adults also display the positivity effect: a tendency for older adults to pay more attention to, better remember, and place more priority on positive information than on negative information

Emotion regulation

  • Emotional experiences of younger and older adults are quite similar however, older adults are skilled at emotion regulation, especially at achieving their main emotion regulation goal: maximizing positive emotions and minimizing negative ones

Bowlby's Attachment theory

  • attachment is a strong affectional tie that binds a person to an intimate companion
  • Attachments unfold through an interaction of biological (Nature) and environmental forces (Nurture) during a sensitive period early in life.
  • The quality of the attachment between infant and caregiver
    shapes later development and the quality of later relationships.
  • The capacity to form attachments is part of our evolutionary heritage.
  • Internal working models of self and others are the mechanism through which early experience affects later development.
  • Internal working models—cognitive representations of themselves and other people that guide their processing of social information and their behavior in relationships
  • Evident through Imprinting, an innate form of learning in which the young will follow and become attached to a moving object (usually the mother) during a critical period early in life to ensure survivability.
  • A peer is a social equal, someone who functions at a similar level of behavioral complexity—often someone of similar age

Peers are equals rather than powerful authority figures, they help children

  • learn that relationships are reciprocal
  • force them to hone their perspective-taking skills
  • provide alternative perspectives on social cognitive and moral development

Social needs change as we get older

  • The parent–child relationship is central up to about age 6
  • Then children need playmates;
  • Then they need acceptance by the peer group; and
  • Then around age 9 to age 12 they most need intimacy in the form of a close friendship (Chumships).

Caregiver’s Attachment to the Infant

  • Parents often begin to form emotional attachments to their babies even before they are born.

Infant’s Attachment to the Caregiver

  • According to Ainsworth and Bowlby, infants progress through four phases in forming attachments

Attachment-Related Fears

Exploratory Behavior

  • Attachment to a caregiver also has the
    positive effect of facilitating exploratory behavior
  • Attachment figure
    serves as a:

Synchronised routines

  • Over time, caregivers and infants develop synchronised routines in which they take turns to respond to each other’s leads.
  • E.g. Mother and infant play “peekaboo”, mother sits back and watches when
    infant is no longer interested, re-engages when infant looks at her again
  • Achieving synchrony between parent and infant makes secure attachment more likely

2. Discriminating social responsiveness

  • (2 or 3 months to 6 or 7 months)
  • Infants begin to show preferences for familiar companions.
  • Still friendly, but exceptionally enthusiastic with familiar companions.

3. Active proximity seeking or true attachment

  • (6 or 7 months to about 3 years)
  • infants form their first clear attachments to the primary care-giver.
  • Proximity-seeking behaviour (i.e. seen crawling/following their primary care-giver)

1. Undiscriminating social responsiveness

  • (birth to 2-3 months)
  • Responsive to and interested in any human, no clear preference for one person

4. Goal-corrected partnership

  • (3 years and older)
  • Takes into account parent’s goals and plans, adjusts behaviour to achieve the goal of optimal proximity
  • i.e. 1-year-old cries and tries to follow when Dad leaves for work, whereas a 4-year-old can understand where Dad is going and wait for his return

Separation Anxiety

  • Appears when infants are forming their first true attachments, peaks between 14-18 months of age before gradually declining
  • Once attached to a parent, baby often becomes fretful when separated from that parent

Stranger Anxiety

  • Emerges shortly after attachment is formed
  • Wary or fretful reaction to the approach of an unfamiliar person
  • Common between 8 and 10 months of age and declines during the second year of life
  • Secure base for exploration—a point of safety from which an infant can feel free to venture
  • Safe haven to which the infant can return for comfort if frightened

Strange Situation

  • A procedure for measuring the quality of an attachment developed by Ainsworth.
  • It consists of eight episodes that gradually escalate the amount of stress infants experience as they react to the approach of an adult stranger and two departures and returns of their caregiver

1. Secure attachment

  • The securely attached infant:
    • proximity maintenance - stays close and continuously monitors the caregiver’s whereabouts
    • safe haven - retreats to her for comfort if needed
    • separation distress - resists and is distressed by separations from her
    • secure base - explores happily as long as caregiver is present and attentive
  • Infants may have parents who are sensitive and responsive to their needs and emotional signals

2. Insecure attachments

2b. Avoidant attachment

  • Infants with avoidant attachments are not very adventuresome, shows little apparent distress/separation anxiety when separated from their mothers, and avoid contact or seem indifferent when their mothers return.
  • not particularly wary of strangers but sometimes avoid or ignore them, much as these babies avoid or ignore their mothers.
  • Poor emotional competency - avoidant infants seem to have shut down their emotions and distanced themselves from their parent.
  • Infants may have parents who tend to provide too much or too little stimulation; either unresponsive or overzealous

2c. Disorganized-disoriented attachment

  • Emotional/behavioural responses appear to be confused.
  • Does not display consistent traits of the other attachment styles.
  • associated with later emotional problems
  • common in infants who have been physically abused, or whose mothers are severely depressed or on drugs/alcohol

2a. Anxious/Ambivalent attachment or Resistant attachment

  • The resistant infant does not dare venture off to play even when her mother is present; her mother does not seem to serve as a secure base for exploration.
  • The infant becomes distressed when her mother departs, often showing strong separation anxiety.
  • The infant is ambivalent, Not easily comforted by her mother.
  • Resistant infants are also wary of strangers, even when their mothers are present
  • Infants may have parents who are inconsistent in their caregiving; e.g. mood-dependent, unresponsive parents

Caregiver’s Contributions to Attachment

  • Contact Comfort: According to Harlow, contact comfort are pleasurable tactile sensations provided by a more powerful contributor to attachment.
  • It also promotes human attachments; (As per Harlow’s experiment, feeding has little effect on the quality of infants’ attachments)

Later Development of Securely and
Insecurely Attached Infants

  • Emotion regulation. Secure attachment in infancy is also linked to good emotion regulation and coping skills
  • Social competence. Children who had been securely attached as infants are more able to initiate play activities, are more sensitive to the needs and feelings of other children, and are more popular and socially competent
  • Intellectual competence. Children who were securely attached as infants are described by teachers as more curious, self-directed, and eager to learn than insecurely attached children

Effects of Social Deprivation

Recovering from seperation

  • Infants who go through long-term or permanent separation from a caregiver usually recover
    • once reunited with the caregiver or
    • if the infant is able to maintain or form an attachment with someone else

Prolonged deprivation

  • However, children who had spent 6 months or more deprived from caregiver settings rarely achieved normal levels of cognitive development.
  • Such children tended to be:
    • emotionally withdrawn
    • indiscriminately friendly (disinhibited attachment)
    • or both

Changes in play

  • We can detect two major changes in play between infancy and age 5:
    • it becomes more social,
    • it becomes more imaginative

Play Becomes More Social

  • Parten classified the types of play engaged in by preschool children of different ages into categories arranged from least to most social

Play Becomes More Imaginative

General types of play

  • Object play (stacking blocks, making crafts)
  • Social play (mutual imitation or playing board games)
  • Locomotor play (games of tag or ball)
  • Pretend play (enacting roles)
  • Parallel play. Children play next to one another, doing much the same thing, but they interact little
  • for example, two girls might sit near each other, both drawing pictures, without talking to each other much.
  • Associative play. Children interact by swapping materials, conversing, or following each other’s lead, but they are not united by the same goal
  • for example, the two girls may swap crayons and comment on each other’s drawings as they draw.
  • Solitary play. Children play alone, typically with objects, and appear to be highly involved in what they are doing
  • Cooperative play. Children join forces to achieve a common goal; they act as a pair or group, dividing their labor and coordinating their activities in a meaningful way
  • for example, the two girls collaborate to draw a big mural.
  • Pretend play - play in which one actor, object, or action symbolizes or stands for another
  • Emerges when a child becomes able to use symbols to represent objects; around the age of 1 year
  • Social Pretend Play - play in which children cooperate with others to enact sometimes very sophisticated dramas (i.e. play masa-masak)
  • Rule-governed play School-age children engage less in pretend play and more in rule-governed play
  • E.g. Board games, hide-and-seek, organised sports
  • According to Piaget, this only occurs after children are in the concrete operations stage and are able to cooperate and follow rules

Functions of Play

  • Engaging in lots of pretend play has been linked to better performance on tests of cognitive development, language skills and creativity
  • Social pretend play helps children understand others’ perspectives and hone their social skills
  • Physical play contributes to neural maturation and the development of motor skills

  • Research has defined 5 distinct categories of social status:
  • Neglected – neither liked nor disliked, isolated, invisible to peers
  • Controversial – liked by many but also disliked by many
  • Rejected – rarely liked, often disliked
  • Average – in the middle on both liked and disliked scales
  • Popular – well liked by most, rarely disliked
  • Popularity is affected by:
    • Physical attractiveness
    • Intelligence
    • Ability to regulate emotions
    • Social competence

Importance of peer acceptance

  • Peer acceptance and popularity are important to development:
    • Children who are rejected (particularly because of aggressive behaviour) are likely to continue to be rejected over the schooling years
    • May end up with poor adjustment as a result; low self-esteem, poor social skills, development of negative attitudes towards others
  • Greater social competence
  • Better emotional adjustment
  • Higher self-esteem
  • Fewer behavioural problems
  • Stronger sense of identity
  • Sociometric popularity - being liked by many peers,
  • Perceived popularity - being viewed as someone who has status, power, and visibility in the peer group
  1. In early adolescence, the most popular boys and girls lead the way and form a heterosexual clique.
  1. As less popular teens also form mixed-sex cliques of their own, a new peer-group structure, the crowd, completes its evolution during the high school years. The crowd (i.e. a loose collection of heterosexual cliques with similar characteristics) provides a vehicle for socializing with the other sex through organized social gatherings such as parties.
  1. Boy cliques and girl cliques then begin to interact. Just as parents provide a secure base for peer relationships, same-sex cliques provide a secure base for romantic relationships.
  1. As more and more couples form and the crowd disintegrate in late high school, having served its purpose of bringing boys and girls together.
  1. In late childhood, boys and girls become members of same-sex cliques, or small friendship groups, and have little to do with the other sex.
  1. Status phase. In mid-adolescence, peer approval is what counts; having a romantic relationship, and having it with the “right kind” of partner, is important for the status it brings in the larger peer group
  1. Affection phase. In late adolescence, the focus is finally on the relationship rather than on self-concept or peer status. Romantic relationships become more personal and caring.
  1. Initiation phase. In early adolescence, the focus is on self-concept. That is, coming to see oneself as a person capable of a romantic relationship. This is a time of crushes, posturing, and awkward beginnings.
  1. Bonding phase. In the transition to emerging adulthood, the emotional intimacy achieved in the affection phase may be coupled with a long-term commitment to create a lasting attachment.

CHAPTER 14.6 Emotions, Attachment,
and Social Relationships

The Adult

Romantic Relationships

  • An important developmental task for young adults is to resolve Erikson’s issue of intimacy versus isolation by finding a romantic partner and entering into a committed relationship

Social Networks

  • The trend toward smaller social networks with age after early adulthood can be seen in many ethnic groups, but ethnic group differences are also evident.
  • Shrinking social networks in late adulthood may be forced in part by chronic illness and disability.
  • Older adults actively choose to shrink their social networks to better meet their emotional needs as they realize that little time is left to them

Adult Attachment Styles The four adult attachment styles can also be described in terms of two dimensions of attachment:

  • anxiety i.e. extent of concern about whether partners will be emotionally available and responsive. Correlates with model of self (Low anxiety = positive model of self)
  • avoidance i.e. extent of discomfort being intimate with and depending on a partner. Corelates with model of others (Low avoidance = positive model of others)

Adult Relationships and Adult Development

  • Close attachments to other people are essential to normal cognitive, social and emotional development
  • Quality rather than quantity of social relationships is closely related to person’s sense of well-being and life satisfaction
  • Important to have at least one confidant – trusted companion to whom the individual feels especially attached and with whom thoughts and feelings can be shared
    • Usually the spouse for married adults; quality of marriage is one of the strongest influences on overall life satisfaction
  • Despite the shrinkage of social networks, older adults are satisfied with their emotional lives
  • Two theories can explain this:

Socioemotional selectivity theory

  • According to this view, the perception that one has little time left to live prompts aging adults to put less emphasis on the goal of acquiring information for future use and more emphasis on the goal of fulfilling current emotional needs. As a result, older adults narrow their range of social partners to those who bring them emotional pleasure.

Positivity effect

  • Older adults also display the positivity effect: a tendency for older adults to pay more attention to, better remember, and place more priority on positive information than on negative information

Partner Choice

  • The greatest influence on mate selection is similarity, or homogamy.
  • Once homogamy is assured, people may also look for complementarity— i.e. look for partners who are different from them but who have strengths that compensate for their own weaknesses or otherwise complement their own characteristics.

Sternberg’s Triarchic Theory of Love

  • Stenberg identifies different types of love based on the strength of three components:

Sternberg’s Triarchic components

  • Intimacy involves feelings of warmth, caring, closeness, trust, and respect in the relationship.

Decision/ Commitment

  • Commitment involves first deciding that one loves the other person and then committing to a long-term relationship.
  • Passion involves sexual attraction, romantic feelings, and excitement.

Sternberg's types of love

  • There are different types of love depending on whether each of the three components of love are high or low

Consummate love – high levels of passion, intimacy, and decision/commitment

Companionate love – high intimacy and commitment but not much passion

Preoccupied attachment style

  • Develop from resistant attachment as a child.
  • Desperate for love to feel worthy as a person; worry about abandonment; express anxiety and anger openly
  • High anxiety, low avoidance

Dismissing attachment style

  • Develop from avoidant attachment as a child.
  • Shut out emotions; defend against hurt by avoiding intimacy, dismissing the importance of relationships, and being “compulsively self-reliant”
  • Low anxiety, high avoidance
  • Secure attachment style
  • Develope from Secure attachment style (during childhood period)
  • Healthy balance of attachment and autonomy; freedom to explore
  • Low anxiety, low avoidance

Fearful attachment style

  • Develop from disorganized–disoriented attachment as a child.
  • Need relationships but doubt own worth and fear intimacy; lack a coherent strategy for meeting attachment needs
  • High anxiety, high avoidance
  • Gender roles— the patterns of behaviour that females and males should adopt in a particular society (social-based, gender role norms)
  • Societal norms may generate gender stereotypes, overgeneralized and largely inaccurate beliefs about the characteristics of all males and all females
  • The nurturer gender role (often female) is built on communality (or communion), an orientation that emphasizes connectedness to others and includes traits of emotionality and sensitivity to others
  • The masculine gender role is agency, an orientation toward individual action and achievement that emphasizes traits of dominance, independence, assertiveness, and competitiveness.
  • This focus of agency stems from the male brain’s tendency to systemize, or analyze and explore how things work

  • Social-role theory suggests that differences in the roles that women and men play in society do a lot to create and maintain gender stereotypes.
  • For example, men have traditionally occupied powerful roles in business. As a result, society paints a picture of man as being dominant and authoritative.

The Child

Acquiring gender stereotypes

  • Through the process of gender typing - tendencies to favour gender-exclusive activities (playing with dolls. Etc), children not only become aware that they are biological males or females but also acquire the motives, values, and patterns of behaviour that their culture considers appropriate for members of their biological sex. (i.e. gender stereo-types)

Acquiring gender identity - Children form gender identity or an awareness that they are either a boy or a girl, by age 2½ to age 3 through gender categorisation reinforced by differential treatment.

gendered based behaviours

  • As gender-typing becomes more salient, children engages in gender segregation. Occurs during elementary school

theory of gender-role development

  • Biosocial theory of gender-role development, states that early biological developments influence how people react to a child, such social reactions have much to do with children’s assuming gender roles.
  • i.e. The biosocial theory predicts that if a biological male were consistently labelled and treated as a girl, he would, by about age 3, acquire the gender identity of a girl.

Social learning theorists argue that children learn masculine or feminine identities, preferences, and behaviours through two processes.

  • First, through differential reinforcement, children are rewarded for sex-appropriate behaviours and are punished for behaviours considered more appropriate for members of the other sex.
  • Second, through observational learning, children adopt the attitudes and behaviors of same-sex models.
  • Kohlberg cognitive theory of gender typing has two major themes:
    • Gender-role development depends on stage-like changes in cognitive development; children must acquire certain understandings about gender before they will be influenced by their social experiences.
    • Children engage in self-socialization; instead of being the passive targets of social influence, they actively socialize themselves.

Identity/stability/consistancy
Kohlberg believes that children progress through three steps as they acquire gender constancy

  • Basic gender identity is established by age 2 or 2½, when children can recognize and label themselves as males or females
  • At around age 3—children acquire gender stability—that is, they come to understand that gender identity is stable over time. They know that boys invariably become men and girls grow up to be women
  • When children achieve gender consistency and realize that their sex is also stable across situations. Now children know that their sex cannot be altered by superficial changes